[wordup] On the evolution of cooking and the self-domestication
Adam Shand
ashand at wetafx.co.nz
Fri May 28 00:19:09 EDT 2004
This possibly the most fascinating thing I've ever read ... there's a
bunch more to the article which is worth reading, I've only included
one section below.
Adam.
From: http://www.edge.org/3rd_culture/wrangham/wrangham_print.html
Via: http://www.livejournal.com/users/jwz/343327.html
...<snip>...
There are two really fascinating things about human evolution that we
have yet to really fully come to grips with. One is the evolution of
cooking. Whenever cooking happened, it must have had absolutely
monstrous effects on us, because cooking enormously increases the
quality of the food we eat, and it enormously increases the range of
food items that we can eat. We all know that food quality and food
abundance are key variables in understanding animal ecology. But the
amazing thing is that although at the moment there is no conventional
wisdom that says when cooking evolved, social anthropology and all
sorts of conventional wisdom say that humans are the animals that cook.
We distinguish ourselves from the rest of the world because they eat
raw stuff and we eat cooked stuff. The best anthropology can do at the
moment is to say that maybe sometime around 250 or 300 thousand years
ago cooking really got going, because there's archeological evidence of
earth ovens.
This is fine, but long before earth ovens came along we must have
learned to cook. And you would think that cooking would be associated
with things like evidence in your body of the food being easier to
digest, such as smaller teeth, or maybe a reduction in the size of the
rib cage as the size of the stomach gets smaller, or maybe the jaw
getting smaller. And there's only one time in human evolution that all
that happens; that is, 1.9 million years ago with the evolution of the
genus Homo. It's there we must look for evidence that cooking was
adopted.
Once cooking happens, it completely changes the way the animal exploits
its environment, because instead of moving from food patch to food
patch, and eating as it goes, or eating in the food patches it finds,
now for the first time it has to accumulate food, put it somewhere, and
sit with it until it's cooked. It might take 20 minutes, it might take
half an hour, it might take several hours. The effect is that all of a
sudden there's a stealable food patch. Once you have a stealable food
patch, that means that — life being what it is — somebody's going to
come along and try to steal it. What this means is that you have to
think about a producer/scrounger dynamic in which you've got
individuals producing and individuals scrounging — and, horribly,
females were the producers and males were the scroungers. Once you've
got males bigger than females — fifty percent bigger by the time we're
talking about, around two million years ago — then the effects on the
social system would be large.
What we've got to think about is the idea that once you have females
ready to make a meal by collecting food and cooking it, then they're
vulnerable to having their food taken away by the scroungers — the big
males — who find it easier not to go and collect food themselves or
cook it, but just take it once it's ready. Therefore the females need
protective bonds in order to protect themselves from thieving males,
and this is the origin of human male-female relationships. The
evolution of cooking is a huge topic that is virtually completely
neglected. And whatever view you take about cooking, you have to say
it's a problem that needs to be addressed.
The second problem is this: There is in a number of ways in the
evolution of humans evidence of our behaving and looking as if we had
the characteristics of a juvenile animal. For a hundred years or more
people have talked about the idea that humans might be a pedomorphic
species, a species that has juvenile characteristics in general, but
this is too global a way to think about it. Still, it remains the case
that much in our behavior, when compared with the behavior of our
closest relatives, looks more playful and less aggressive when you're
thinking about interactions at a social level within a group. We are
also more sexual and more ready to learn, and these sorts of
characteristics are characteristics generally associated with
juvenility.
In a fascinating parallel, the bonobos — the second in the great pair
of our two closest relatives — show all sorts of traits that are
pedomorphic. We can see this throughout the head, where the morphology
of the skull itself looks like the skull of an early adolescent or
late-juvenile chimpanzee and much of its behavior looks juvenile-like.
They are more playful, they're less sex-differentiated in all sorts of
aspects of their behavior, they're more sexual, and so on. And we've
yet to really come to grips with where this pedomorphic change has come
from and what it means.
We've already got some wonderful examples of similar things occuring in
other animals in the context of domestication. When we look at the
differences between wolves and dogs, for example, we see amazing
parallels to the differences between chimpanzees and bonobos. In each
case for a given size of animal, you have the skull being reduced in
size, and the components of the skull being reduced in size, including
the jaws and teeth, and the skull looking more like the skull of a
juvenile in the other form. The dog's skull looks like that of a
juvenile wolf, and the bonobo's skull looks like that of a juvenile
chimpanzee. And the behavior of each of them looks like it has strong
components of the juvenile of the other species.
This leads to the thought that species can self-domesticate. There is
good reason to think that over the course of evolution the bonobos
evolved from a chimpanzee-like ancestor as a consequence of being in an
environment where aggression was less beneficial to the aggressors,
where there was a natural selection against aggression, and where
selection favored individuals that were less aggressive. Over time,
selection built on those slight variations in the timing of the arrival
of the aggressive characteristics in the adult males. So it was
constantly pushing back, favoring individuals that retained more
juvenile-like behavior — and even juvenile-like heads — because that's
what controls the behavior. Later, what you had was a species that had
effectively been tamed, had been self-domesticated.
There is experimental evidence of this process. We have the Russian
geneticist Belyaev, for example, who actually took wild foxes, selected
for purely tame traits over 20 or 30 generations, and at the end of
that time observed not only that the descendant foxes are as tame as
dogs are nowadays — spontaneously — but also that they have a series of
characteristics that have come along for the ride, incidental
consequences that were not selected for but are just there. You have
dramatic morphological ones, like the star mutation — the white spot on
the forehead that you see in horses and cows and goats — that are just
somehow associated genetically with tameness, and probably result from
some kind of change in developmental events. There are also other
morphological changes, like curly hair, short tails, and lopped ears,
which happened in a number of domesticated animals, apparently because
they've been selected for tameness. In addition, you get these smaller
brains.
There is a remarkable thing about human evolution. We always tend to
think that humans have just had a continuous surge in brain size over
the last two million years, but actually over the last thirty thousand
years brain size has decreased by 10 to 15 percent. The standard
explanation for this is that we became more gracile at the same time —
that we became thinner boned — which meant that we were lighter in body
weight. And because there tends to be a correlation between body weight
and brain weight, then maybe this explains our smaller brains. But I
don't see any reason why brain size should be correlated with the
amount of meat we carry on our bodies. This gracility is exactly the
same pattern we see in the evolution of dogs from wolves, or bonobos
from chimpanzees, or domesticated foxes from wild foxes. In all these
cases an increasing gracility of the bone is an incidental effect.
I think that we have to start thinking about the idea that humans in
the last 30, 40, or 50 thousand years have been domesticating
ourselves. If we’re following the bonobo or dog pattern, we're moving
toward a form of ourselves with more and more juvenile behavior. And
the amazing thing once you start thinking in these terms is that you
realize that we're still moving fast. Tooth size, for example, is
extremely strongly genetically controlled and develops with little
environmental influence, and is continuing to decline fast. I think
that current evidence is that we're in the middle of an evolutionary
event in which tooth size is falling, jaw size is falling, brain size
is falling, and it's quite reasonable to imagine that we're continuing
to tame ourselves. The way it's happening is the way it's probably
happened since we became permanently settled in villages, 20 or 30
thousand years ago, or before.
People who are anti-social, for example, have their breeding
opportunities reduced. They may be executed, they may be imprisoned, or
they may be punished so badly that they're kept out of the breeding
pool. Just as there is selection for tameness in the domestication
process of wild animals, or just as in bonobos there was a natural
selection against aggressiveness, here there's a sort of social
selection against excessively aggressive people within communities.
This puts humans in a picture of now undergoing a process of becoming
increasingly a peaceful form of a more aggressive ancestor.
...<snip>...
More information about the wordup
mailing list